WEBVTT 00:00:00.000 --> 00:00:02.300 align:middle line:90% Hi, Werner. 00:00:02.300 --> 00:00:03.500 align:middle line:90% Hello, Greta. 00:00:03.500 --> 00:00:09.030 align:middle line:84% In pupillometry, people often use baselines. 00:00:09.030 --> 00:00:11.150 align:middle line:90% What are those? 00:00:11.150 --> 00:00:11.650 align:middle line:90% Yeah. 00:00:11.650 --> 00:00:14.770 align:middle line:84% So we use baselines because there 00:00:14.770 --> 00:00:18.460 align:middle line:84% are a lot of things that we don't control 00:00:18.460 --> 00:00:20.290 align:middle line:90% when we measure pupil sizes. 00:00:20.290 --> 00:00:21.950 align:middle line:90% But we can control that. 00:00:21.950 --> 00:00:24.790 align:middle line:90% So there are these things like-- 00:00:24.790 --> 00:00:28.690 align:middle line:84% for example, different people have different pupil sizes. 00:00:28.690 --> 00:00:31.870 align:middle line:84% You might have slightly different default pupil size 00:00:31.870 --> 00:00:36.460 align:middle line:84% than I do, or it can be reactive in a slightly different way. 00:00:36.460 --> 00:00:40.600 align:middle line:84% And then pictures or videos, or any stimuli 00:00:40.600 --> 00:00:44.260 align:middle line:84% that we might want to show our study participants, 00:00:44.260 --> 00:00:47.920 align:middle line:84% they also might have different luminance or brightness, 00:00:47.920 --> 00:00:49.450 align:middle line:90% different visual properties. 00:00:49.450 --> 00:00:51.230 align:middle line:90% They change a lot. 00:00:51.230 --> 00:00:57.400 align:middle line:84% So we use baselines to basically control for all these factors 00:00:57.400 --> 00:00:59.140 align:middle line:90% that might vary. 00:00:59.140 --> 00:01:02.980 align:middle line:84% In our experiment, it became a common practise to first take 00:01:02.980 --> 00:01:05.890 align:middle line:84% the baseline measurement of our pupil, 00:01:05.890 --> 00:01:11.000 align:middle line:84% and then take the measurement from our target stimulus. 00:01:11.000 --> 00:01:11.500 align:middle line:90% OK. 00:01:11.500 --> 00:01:13.480 align:middle line:90% So let me understand. 00:01:13.480 --> 00:01:16.430 align:middle line:90% So the baseline is an image. 00:01:16.430 --> 00:01:21.170 align:middle line:84% And is there an image that works for all experiments, 00:01:21.170 --> 00:01:24.570 align:middle line:84% or you have to adjust to each situation? 00:01:24.570 --> 00:01:25.070 align:middle line:90% Yeah. 00:01:25.070 --> 00:01:26.720 align:middle line:84% So it depends on your experiment, right? 00:01:26.720 --> 00:01:27.762 align:middle line:90% It depends on your study. 00:01:27.762 --> 00:01:33.140 align:middle line:84% If, in your study, you show your study participants 00:01:33.140 --> 00:01:35.870 align:middle line:84% static images, then your baseline 00:01:35.870 --> 00:01:37.880 align:middle line:90% can also be a static image-- 00:01:37.880 --> 00:01:42.140 align:middle line:84% that is, for example, a blurred or pixelated version 00:01:42.140 --> 00:01:44.820 align:middle line:84% of what you want to show to your participants. 00:01:44.820 --> 00:01:48.560 align:middle line:84% So in this way, you are maintaining all the visual 00:01:48.560 --> 00:01:52.040 align:middle line:84% properties-- all the luminance and brightness, colours-- 00:01:52.040 --> 00:01:55.760 align:middle line:84% of your target stimulus that you're showing, 00:01:55.760 --> 00:01:58.400 align:middle line:84% but you're taking away the meaning 00:01:58.400 --> 00:02:01.350 align:middle line:84% or the content of what you're showing to a participant. 00:02:01.350 --> 00:02:01.850 align:middle line:90% OK. 00:02:01.850 --> 00:02:02.350 align:middle line:90% I got that. 00:02:02.350 --> 00:02:08.669 align:middle line:84% But then, when would you show this baseline image? 00:02:08.669 --> 00:02:09.169 align:middle line:90% Yeah. 00:02:09.169 --> 00:02:14.540 align:middle line:84% So typically, you show it right before the target image 00:02:14.540 --> 00:02:15.560 align:middle line:90% that you want to show. 00:02:15.560 --> 00:02:18.560 align:middle line:84% But you don't have to show it for the exact same amount 00:02:18.560 --> 00:02:19.550 align:middle line:90% of time. 00:02:19.550 --> 00:02:21.620 align:middle line:84% You can show it for a shorter time-- for example, 00:02:21.620 --> 00:02:24.920 align:middle line:90% one second or slightly shorter. 00:02:24.920 --> 00:02:31.160 align:middle line:84% That said, also remember that pupillary responses 00:02:31.160 --> 00:02:32.370 align:middle line:90% are pretty slow. 00:02:32.370 --> 00:02:32.870 align:middle line:90% Right? 00:02:32.870 --> 00:02:37.620 align:middle line:84% So we need to allow the pupil some time to adjust. 00:02:37.620 --> 00:02:39.920 align:middle line:84% So if you would like to show the baseline for just 00:02:39.920 --> 00:02:42.590 align:middle line:84% a few milliseconds, that's probably too short. 00:02:42.590 --> 00:02:44.900 align:middle line:84% So it should be for around a second. 00:02:44.900 --> 00:02:45.500 align:middle line:90% OK. 00:02:45.500 --> 00:02:48.890 align:middle line:84% And then you get a measurement of the pupil 00:02:48.890 --> 00:02:50.330 align:middle line:90% during the baseline. 00:02:50.330 --> 00:02:50.900 align:middle line:90% Mm-hmm. 00:02:50.900 --> 00:02:54.340 align:middle line:90% So how do you use those data? 00:02:54.340 --> 00:02:54.840 align:middle line:90% Yeah. 00:02:54.840 --> 00:03:00.050 align:middle line:84% So what you get, what you measure from the baseline 00:03:00.050 --> 00:03:03.620 align:middle line:84% image, you would usually subtract 00:03:03.620 --> 00:03:07.440 align:middle line:84% that-- that average pupil size, collected from the baseline 00:03:07.440 --> 00:03:07.940 align:middle line:90% image. 00:03:07.940 --> 00:03:11.840 align:middle line:84% You would subtract that from the average pupil size 00:03:11.840 --> 00:03:14.270 align:middle line:84% that you collected at your target stimulus, 00:03:14.270 --> 00:03:17.600 align:middle line:84% at the actual picture that you want participants to see. 00:03:17.600 --> 00:03:21.860 align:middle line:84% And in this way, you can have that difference 00:03:21.860 --> 00:03:27.260 align:middle line:84% of how the pupil changed from the baseline 00:03:27.260 --> 00:03:30.590 align:middle line:84% while you were looking at the target image. 00:03:30.590 --> 00:03:35.270 align:middle line:84% And this shows us that magnitude of pupil response. 00:03:35.270 --> 00:03:39.380 align:middle line:84% And it's normalised across your research subjects. 00:03:39.380 --> 00:03:43.190 align:middle line:84% And it's irrespective of their individual pupil size, 00:03:43.190 --> 00:03:46.670 align:middle line:84% or the individual properties of the stimuli-- 00:03:46.670 --> 00:03:48.510 align:middle line:90% like their luminance, right? 00:03:48.510 --> 00:03:49.010 align:middle line:90% OK. 00:03:49.010 --> 00:03:51.720 align:middle line:84% So it's correcting that information. 00:03:51.720 --> 00:03:52.220 align:middle line:90% Exactly. 00:03:52.220 --> 00:03:53.678 align:middle line:84% And this is basically what we want. 00:03:53.678 --> 00:03:55.220 align:middle line:90% That's an important thing to do. 00:03:55.220 --> 00:03:56.970 align:middle line:90% Exactly. 00:03:56.970 --> 00:03:58.800 align:middle line:90% Thank you, Greta. 00:03:58.800 --> 00:04:01.900 align:middle line:84% Now we understand much better what's our baseline. 00:04:01.900 --> 00:04:04.280 align:middle line:90% So I hope everyone understands. 00:04:04.280 --> 00:04:06.070 align:middle line:90% Thank you. 00:04:06.070 --> 00:04:17.000 align:middle line:90%